Social Learning Lab
Stanford University | Spring 2019
↞ About Us ↠
Children are powerful learners.
Right from the beginning, they quickly figure out “how the world works.” They seem to do so with little effort.
But how do they do it?
At the Social Learning Lab, we are on a mission to find out. Social learning—how humans learn from other humans—especially interests us. To study how children behave in social situations, our researchers bring fun games to our partner museums and schools.
See what we’ve been up to!
Hyowon Gweon | Lab Director
Dept. of Psychology
Stanford University
PhD in Cognitive Science @ MIT (2012)
↞ Featured Project 1 ↠
Griffin Dietz | Graduate Student
I am a doctoral student in Computer Science interested in the way that children's interactions and education are mediated and affected by technology. Currently, I am studying how child development should inform the way we teach computational thinking to young children. Specifically, how can we separate problem solving from programming to make Computer Science Education accessible to younger children?
Email: gdietz44@stanford.edu
Breaking Down Complex Problems
↞ W H A T ↠
To solve a problem in computer science, you have to break it into parts, choose solutions to those parts, and combine those solutions back together. By preschool, children can reassemble solutions to block building problems given the correct problems, but can they choose the best parts to begin with?
↞ H O W ↠
We had children (age: 3 – 5) solve complex block building problems by having them select the block pieces they needed to make a bridge – importantly, the bridge had to stand up by itself!
↞ A N D ↠
We found that older children (age: 4 & 5) were reliably able to select the best option (below, right.) They are generally successful at the decomposition and reconstruction process that is so important to computer science! 3-year-olds, however, did not favor the correct solution over the incorrect one; they were essentially guessing.
↞ W H Y ↠
Preschoolers are still developing the ability to plan ahead in order to solve problems. To solve this problem, the children need to not only evaluate the options they have now, but what those options might look like in the future if they were to put the blocks together.
Nonetheless these findings still show promise in children’s ability to begin learning computational thinking when they start Kindergarten.
↞ Featured Project 2 ↠
Yang Wu | Postdoctoral Scholar
Emotional cues are abundant in our daily lives. How do young children make sense of these cues? In my research, I study how infants and children use observed emotional signals to reason about the unknown world and to guide their learning and exploration. These abilities, I propose, are supported by an intuitive theory of emotion that is connected to children’s knowledge of the physical and social world broadly. The work spans methods from infant looking time measures to computational models. It advances our understanding of the remarkable human capacities to learn in social contexts, and bridges gaps across disciplines including developmental, cognitive and affective sciences.
Email: yangwu1@stanford.edu
Using Emotions to Learn
↞ W H A T ↠
In our study, we looked at whether children can rationally use others’ expressions of surprise to guide their own exploration of a novel toy.
↞ H O W ↠
A researcher showed a novel toy to children (age: 3-4 years) and, while playing together, they found out a function of the toy. Next, the researcher played with the toy out of sight of the child and made a surprised facial expression. Given the surprised expression, we expected the child to think that there was another function of the toy, and explore the toy broadly to search for that function.
The second condition was nearly identical except that the surprised expression came from a naive adult who came into the room. In this case, because the child knew more about the toy than the naive adult, the child might be less likely to use the naive adult’s surprise to guide their own exploration.
↞ A N D ↠
As predicted, we found that children between ages 3 and 4 explored the toy more broadly and persistently following the surprise of the knowledgeable adult, than that of the naive adult.
↞ W H Y ↠
The study shows that children can use others’ emotional expressions selectively and rationally, to guide their own learning and play. We hope to raise parents’ awareness of their own emotional displays, and the role of those displays in guiding early learning.
↞ Ongoing Projects ↠
How can I help? | Sophie Bridgers
Do young children reason about others’ competence when choosing whether and how to help others?
Disclosing Information about the Self | Mika Asaba
When and why do we choose to tell others about our successes and failures?
Shared Preferences | Natalia Vélez
Do children prefer people with rare shared preferences over those who share common preferences?
Decisions with Difficulty | Grace Bennett-Pierre & Mika Asaba
Do children use difficulty information to make helping and efficiency decisions?
Decomposing Problems | Griffin Dietz
Can young children break problems down into manageable parts?
↞ Recent Papers ↠
Bridgers, S., & Gweon, H. (in press). Means-Inference as a source of variability in early helping. Frontiers in Psychology. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01735
Gweon, H., & Asaba, M. (2018). Order matters: Children’s evaluation of under-informative teachers depends on context. Child Development, 89(3),e278-e292. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12825
Gweon, H., Shafto, P. & Schulz, L.E. (2018). Development of children's sensitivity to over-informativeness in learning and teaching. Developmental Psychology. , 54(11), 2113-2125. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev00005802113
Vélez, N., & Gweon, H. (in press). Integrating incomplete information with imperfect advice. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1 - 17. doi: 10.1111/tops.12388
Questions?
Email: sll.stanford@gmail.com
Website: sll.stanford.edu
Location: 450 Serra Mall (Jordan Hall, Building 420), Stanford, CA, United States
Phone: (650) 498-7832